Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

dc.contributor.authorJanssen, Nielsspa
dc.contributor.authorvan der Meij, Maartjespa
dc.contributor.authorLópez-Pérez, Pedro Javierspa
dc.contributor.authorBarber, Horacio A.spa
dc.date.accessioned2020-04-20T16:23:06Z
dc.date.available2020-04-20T16:23:06Z
dc.date.issued2020
dc.identifier.issn2045-2322spa
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/11323/6224spa
dc.description.abstractSpeech production is a complex skill whose neural implementation relies on a large number of different regions in the brain. How neural activity in these different regions varies as a function of time during the production of speech remains poorly understood. Previous MEG studies on this topic have concluded that activity proceeds from posterior to anterior regions of the brain in a sequential manner. Here we tested this claim using the EEG technique. Specifically, participants performed a picture naming task while their naming latencies and scalp potentials were recorded. We performed group temporal Independent Component Analysis (group tICA) to obtain temporally independent component timecourses and their corresponding topographic maps. We identified fifteen components whose estimated neural sources were located in various areas of the brain. The trial-by-trial component timecourses were predictive of the naming latency, implying their involvement in the task. Crucially, we computed the degree of concurrent activity of each component timecourse to test whether activity was sequential or parallel. Our results revealed that these fifteen distinct neural sources exhibit largely concurrent activity during speech production. These results suggest that speech production relies on neural activity that takes place in parallel networks of distributed neural sources.spa
dc.language.isoeng
dc.publisherScientific Reportsspa
dc.rightsCC0 1.0 Universalspa
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/spa
dc.subjectNeural implementationspa
dc.subjectNeural activityspa
dc.subjectMEGspa
dc.subjectTechnique EEGspa
dc.titleExploring the temporal dynamics of speech production with EEG and group ICAspa
dc.typeArtículo de revistaspa
dc.rights.accessrightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/openAccessspa
dc.identifier.doihttps://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-60301-1spa
dc.identifier.instnameCorporación Universidad de la Costaspa
dc.identifier.reponameREDICUC - Repositorio CUCspa
dc.identifier.repourlhttps://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/spa
dc.relation.references1. Bressler, S. L. Large-scale cortical networks and cognition. Brain Research Reviews 20(3), 288–304 (1995).spa
dc.relation.references2. Chartier, J., Anumanchipalli, G. K., Johnson, K. & Chang, E. F. Encoding of articulatory kinematic trajectories in human speech sensorimotor cortex. Neuron 98((5) jun), 1042–1054.e4 (2018).spa
dc.relation.references3. Fried, I., Ojemann, G. A. & Fetz, E. E. Language-related potentials specifc to human language cortex. Science 212(4492), 353–356 (1981).spa
dc.relation.references4. Indefrey, P. & Levelt, W. J. Te spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition 92(1), 101–144 (2004).spa
dc.relation.references5. Jürgens, U. Neural pathways underlying vocal control. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 26(2), 235–258 (2002).spa
dc.relation.references6. Piai, V. et al. Direct brain recordings reveal hippocampal rhythm underpinnings of language processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113(40), 11366–11371 (2016).spa
dc.relation.references7. Simonyan, K. & Horwitz, B. Laryngeal motor cortex and control of speech in humans. Te Neuroscientist 17(feb 2), 197–208 (2011).spa
dc.relation.references8. Etard, O. et al. Picture naming without brocaas and wernickeas area. Neuroreport 11(3), 617–622 (2000).spa
dc.relation.references9. Geranmayeh, F. et al. Te contribution of the inferior parietal cortex to spoken language production. Brain and language 121(1), 47–57 (2012).spa
dc.relation.references10. Janssen, N. & Mendieta, C. C. R. Te dynamics of speech motor control revealed with time-resolved fmri. Cerebral Cortex, (2019).spa
dc.relation.references11. Murtha, S., Chertkow, H., Beauregard, M. & Evans, A. Te neural substrate of picture naming. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 11(4), 399–423 (1999).spa
dc.relation.references12. Price, C. J. A review and synthesis of the frst 20 years of pet and fmri studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. Neuroimage 62(2), 816–847 (2012).spa
dc.relation.references13. Hulten, A., Vihla, M., Laine, M. & Salmelin, R. Accessing newly learned names and meanings in the native language. Human brain mapping 30(3), 976–989 (2009).spa
dc.relation.references14. Liljeström, M., Hulten, A., Parkkonen, L. & Salmelin, R. Comparing meg and fmri views to naming actions and objects. Human brain mapping 30(6), 1845–1856 (2009).spa
dc.relation.references15. Maess, B., Friederici, A. D., Damian, M., Meyer, A. S. & Levelt, W. J. Semantic category interference in overt picture naming: Sharpening current density localization by pca. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 14(3), 455–462 (2002).spa
dc.relation.references16. Salmelin, R., Hari, R., Lounasmaa, O. & Sams, M. Dynamics of brain activation during picture naming. Nature 368(6470), 463–465 (1994).spa
dc.relation.references17. Sörös, P., Cornelissen, K., Laine, M. & Salmelin, R. Naming actions and objects: cortical dynamics in healthy adults and in an anomic patient with a dissociation in action/object naming. Neuroimage 19(4), 1787–1801 (2003).spa
dc.relation.references18. Vihla, M., Laine, M. & Salmelin, R. Cortical dynamics of visual/semantic vs. phonological analysis in picture confrontation. Neuroimage 33(2), 732–738 (2006).spa
dc.relation.references19. Indefrey, P. On putative shortcomings and dangerous future avenues: response to strijkers & costa. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 31(4), 517–520 (2016).spa
dc.relation.references20. Fargier, R. & Laganaro, M. Spatio-temporal dynamics of referential and inferential naming: Diferent brain and cognitive operations to lexical selection. Brain Topography 30(jun 2), 182–197 (2016).spa
dc.relation.references21. Kober, H. et al. New approach to localize speech relevant brain areas and hemispheric dominance using spatially filtered magnetoencephalography. Human brain mapping 14(4), 236–250 (2001).spa
dc.relation.references22. Miozzo, M., Pulvermüller, F. & Hauk, O. Early parallel activation of semantics and phonology in picture naming: Evidence from a multiple linear regression meg study. Cerebral Cortex 25(10), 3343–3355 (2014).spa
dc.relation.references23. Munding, D., Dubarry, A.-S. & Alario, F.-X. On the cortical dynamics of word production: A review of the meg evidence. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 31(4), 441–462 (2016).spa
dc.relation.references24. Rahman, R. A. & Sommer, W. Does phonological encoding in speech production always follow the retrieval of semantic knowledge?: Electrophysiological evidence for parallel processing. Cognitive Brain Research 16(3), 372–382 (2003).spa
dc.relation.references25. Strijkers, K. & Costa, A. Te cortical dynamics of speaking: Present shortcomings and future avenues. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 31(4), 484–503 (2016).spa
dc.relation.references26. Hämäläinen, M., Hari, R., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Knuutila, J. & Lounasmaa, O. V. Magnetoencephalography-theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain. Reviews of modern Physics 65(2), 413 (1993).spa
dc.relation.references27. Delorme, A., Palmer, J., Onton, J., Oostenveld, R. & Makeig, S. Independent eeg sources are dipolar. PloS one 7(2), e30135 (2012).spa
dc.relation.references28. DiRusso, F., Martínez, A., Sereno, M. I., Pitzalis, S. & Hillyard, S. A. Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential. Human brain mapping 15(2), 95–111 (2002).spa
dc.relation.references29. Riès, S., Janssen, N., Burle, B. & Alario, F.-X. Response-locked brain dynamics of word production. PLoS One 8(3), e58197 (2013).spa
dc.relation.references30. Glaser, W. R. Picture naming. Cognition 42(1), 61–105 (1992).spa
dc.relation.references31. Handy, T. C. Event-related potentials: A methods handbook. MIT press, (2005).spa
dc.relation.references32. Costa, A., Strijkers, K., Martin, C. & Tierry, G. Te time course of word retrieval revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106(50), 21442–21446 (2009).spa
dc.relation.references33. Szekely, A. et al. A new on-line resource for psycholinguistic studies. Journal of memory and language 51(2), 247–250 (2004).spa
dc.relation.references34. Delorme, A. & Makeig, S. Eeglab: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial eeg dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of neuroscience methods 134(1), 9–21 (2004).spa
dc.relation.references35. Winkler, I., Debener, S., Müller, K.-R. & Tangermann, M. On the infuence of high-pass fltering on ica-based artifact reduction in eeg-erp. In Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE, pages 4101–4105. IEEE, (2015).spa
dc.relation.references36. Acunzo, D. J., MacKenzie, G. & van Rossum, M. C. Systematic biases in early erp and erf components as a result of high-pass fltering. Journal of neuroscience methods 209(1), 212–218 (2012).spa
dc.relation.references37. Maess, B., Schröger, E. & Widmann, A. High-pass filters and baseline correction in m/eeg analysis. commentary on:how inappropriate high-pass flters can produce artefacts and incorrect conclusions in erp studies of language and cognition. Journal of neuroscience methods 266, 164–165 (2016).spa
dc.relation.references38. Tanner, D., Morgan-Short, K. & Luck, S. J. How inappropriate high-pass filters can produce artifactual effects and incorrect conclusions in erp studies of language and cognition. Psychophysiology 52(8), 997–1009 (2015).spa
dc.relation.references39. Widmann, A., Schröger, E. & Maess, B. Digital flter design for electrophysiological data-a practical approach. Journal of neuroscience methods 250, 34–46 (2015).spa
dc.relation.references40. Porcaro, C., Medaglia, M. T. & Krott, A. Removing speech artifacts from electroencephalographic recordings during overt picture naming. NeuroImage 105, 171–180 (2015).spa
dc.relation.references41. Comon, P. Independent component analysis, a new concept? Signal processing 36(3), 287–314 (1994).spa
dc.relation.references42. Petersen, K., Hansen, L. K., Kolenda, T., Rostrup, E. & Strother, S. On the independent components of functional neuroimages. In Tird international conference on independent component analysis and blind source separation, pages 615–620 (2000).spa
dc.relation.references43. Makeig, S. et al. Dynamic brain sources of visual evoked responses. Science 295(5555), 690–694 (2002).spa
dc.relation.references44. Hyvarinen, A. Fast and robust fxed-point algorithms for independent component analysis. IEEE transactions on Neural Networks 10(3), 626–634 (1999).spa
dc.relation.references45. Kovacevic, N. & McIntosh, A. R. Groupwise independent component decomposition of eeg data and partial least square analysis. Neuroimage 35(3), 1103–1112 (2007).spa
dc.relation.references46. Makeig, S. et al. Electroencephalographic brain dynamics following manually responded visual targets. PLoS biology 2(6), e176 (2004).spa
dc.relation.references47. Artoni, F., Delorme, A. & Makeig, S. Applying dimension reduction to EEG data by principal component analysis reduces the quality of its subsequent independent component decomposition. NeuroImage 175(jul), 176–187 (2018).spa
dc.relation.references48. Makeig, S., Debener, S., Onton, J. & Delorme, A. Mining event-related brain dynamics. Trends in cognitive sciences 8(5), 204–210 (2004).spa
dc.relation.references49. Desikan, R. S. et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on mri scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31(3), 968–980 (2006).spa
dc.relation.references50. Delorme, A., Sejnowski, T. & Makeig, S. Enhanced detection of artifacts in eeg data using higher-order statistics and independent component analysis. Neuroimage 34(4), 1443–1449 (2007).spa
dc.relation.references51. Joyce, C. A., Gorodnitsky, I. F. & Kutas, M. Automatic removal of eye movement and blink artifacts from eeg data using blind component separation. Psychophysiology 41(2), 313–325 (2004).spa
dc.relation.references52. Weber, M. J. & Tompson-Schill, S. L. Functional neuroimaging can support causal claims about brain function. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 22(11), 2415–2416 (2010).spa
dc.relation.references53. Janssen, N., Hernández-Cabrera, J. A., van der Meij, M. & Barber, H. A. Tracking the time course of competition during word production: Evidence for a post-retrieval mechanism of confict resolution. Cerebral Cortex 25(9), 2960–2969 (2014).spa
dc.relation.references54. Guthrie, D. & Buchwald, J. S. Signifcance testing of diference potentials. Psychophysiology 28(2), 240–244 (1991).spa
dc.relation.references55. Protopapas, A. Check vocal: A program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from dmdx. Behavior Research Methods 39(4), 859–862 (2007).spa
dc.relation.references56. Delorme, A., Miyakoshi, M., Jung, T.-P. & Makeig, S. Grand average erp-image plotting and statistics: A method for comparing variability in event-related single-trial eeg activities across subjects and conditions. Journal of neuroscience methods 250, 3–6 (2015).spa
dc.relation.references57. Geranmayeh, F., Wise, R. J. S., Mehta, A. & Leech, R. Overlapping networks engaged during spoken language production and its cognitive control. Journal of Neuroscience 34(jun 26), 8728–8740 (2014).spa
dc.relation.references58. Riecker, A. et al. fmri reveals two distinct cerebral networks subserving speech motor control. Neurology 64(4), 700–706 (2005).spa
dc.relation.references59. Hassan, M. et al. Dynamic reorganization of functional brain networks during picture naming. Cortex 73, 276–288 (2015).spa
dc.relation.references60. Song, J. et al. Eeg source localization: sensor density and head surface coverage. Journal of neuroscience methods 256, 9–21 (2015).spa
dc.relation.references61. Cisek, P. & Kalaska, J. F. Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annual review of neuroscience 33, 269–298 (2010).spa
dc.relation.references62. Ledberg, A., Bressler, S. L., Ding, M., Coppola, R. & Nakamura, R. Large-scale visuomotor integration in the cerebral cortex. Cerebral cortex 17(1), 44–62 (2006).spa
dc.relation.references63. Liang, H., Bressler, S. L., Ding, M., Truccolo, W. A. & Nakamura, R. Synchronized activity in prefrontal cortex during anticipation of visuomotor processing. Neuroreport 13(16), 2011–2015 (2002).spa
dc.relation.references64. Fetz, E. E. Are movement parameters recognizably coded in the activity of single neurons? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15, 679–690 (1992).spa
dc.relation.references65. Smith, S. M. et al. Functional connectomics from resting-state fmri. Trends in cognitive sciences 17(12), 666–682 (2013).spa
dc.relation.references66. Fries, P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(oct 10), 474–480 (2005).spa
dc.relation.references67. Kornhuber, H. H. & Deecke, L. Brain potential changes in voluntary and passive movements in humans: readiness potential and reaferent potentials. Pfügers Archiv-European Journal of Physiology 468(7), 1115–1124 (2016).spa
dc.relation.references68. Nickerson, L. D., Smith, S. M., Öngür, D. & Beckmann, C. F. Using dual regression to investigate network shape and amplitude in functional connectivity analyses. Frontiers in Neuroscience (11, mar 2017).spa
dc.type.coarhttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501spa
dc.type.contentTextspa
dc.type.driverinfo:eu-repo/semantics/articlespa
dc.type.redcolhttp://purl.org/redcol/resource_type/ARTspa
dc.type.versioninfo:eu-repo/semantics/acceptedVersionspa
dc.type.coarversionhttp://purl.org/coar/version/c_ab4af688f83e57aaspa
dc.rights.coarhttp://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2spa


Ficheros en el ítem

Thumbnail
Thumbnail

Este ítem aparece en la(s) siguiente(s) colección(ones)

  • Artículos científicos [3120]
    Artículos de investigación publicados por miembros de la comunidad universitaria.

Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

CC0 1.0 Universal
Excepto si se señala otra cosa, la licencia del ítem se describe como CC0 1.0 Universal